Most evolution scientists assert that human language began 200,000
to 300,000 years ago and they use speech development as a defining feature, if
not the defining feature of homo sapiens. People, of course, could
communicate via body language and physical signaling of various kinds at some
undefined period, as well. And pictorial
communication, such as by cave paintings, are conventionally dated to about
30,000 years ago.
Before 1844, real time talk required real time physical,
in-person interaction. That meant that
people were consciously or unconsciously perceiving not only the literal
content of their verbalizations but also their body language and prosody (vocal
pitch, length of sounds, loudness, and timber [quality of the voice]). Ancient wisdom acknowledges the importance of
body language and prosody as expressed in the advice, “It’s not what you say
but how you say it.”
Not until the invention of the telegraph approximately in 1844
could people could do anything close to real-time “talking.” We all now are acutely aware of and
preoccupied with our ability to talk to almost anyone on earth at virtually any
time. Progress? Yes and no.
The contemporary telephone that provides real-time vocal
conversation also makes texting possible as an alternative to vocal talk. We all know the misunderstandings that can
arise when texting. When texting, there obviously
is no way to confidently perceive the body language or prosody that face-to-face
conversation enables, especially critical communication nuances, such as facetiousness
or sarcasm.
Whether used for vocal talk or texting, however, the mobile
phone has profoundly changed the dynamics of human communication. Each contact attempt invites the sender and
the target to evaluate the quality of their relationship according to the
frequency and rapidity of sending and receiving. Both communicators commonly presume that the
message has been delivered and that their counterpart virtually has total
control over deciding the next step. Thus,
as expected, Emma M. Templeton and associates (2022)
found that people who received faster responses regarded themselves as “more
connected” to their response partner than did those with slower responses. Also, unsurprisingly, people who response prized
quick responses from their response partner did not necessarily believe that their
own speed was as important. One might
infer that such people were operating according to the “fundamental attribution
error” (Miller, J. G., 1984), finding reasonable external excuses for their own
delayed responses but believing that the speed of others’ responses were determined
by enduring personality characteristics of those others. It is worth mentioning, too, the Templeton
group’s belief that extremely short response times, such as those less than 250
milliseconds, are made without conscious control and thus are considered to be an
honest signal as to how well and easily two people relate to one another.
Consider the “texted” information that I presented within
this blog when you are deciding how and when to communicate, particularly when
interpersonal and/or other adaptive issues are important to you.
References:
Miller, J. G.
(1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 961–978.
Templeton, E. M., et al. (2022). Fast response times signal social connection in conversation.
PNAS January 25. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2116915119.
No comments:
Post a Comment